By Kevin E. Noonan --
Early last week, the Journal of the American Medical Association published a study ("Interaction Between the Serotonin Transporter Gene (5-HTTLPR), Stressful Life Events, and Risk of Depression") by Neil Risch at the University of California, San Francisco and colleagues at the National Institutes of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins University, Yale University School of Medicine, the Rockefeller University, the University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, and Virginia Commonwealth University that showed no statistically-significant link between depression and the short HTT allele. The study provided a statistical meta-analysis of data accumulated for 14,250 participants from 14 separate studies from the medical literature and a second meta-analysis of 10,943 participants from 10 of these studies. These results were contrary to the results published by Avshalom Caspi and collaborators (including Terrie E. Moffitt (at right), Knut Schmidt Nielsen Professor, Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, and Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University) that individuals bearing at least one copy of a specific genetic polymorphism in a serotonin transporter gene were more susceptible to depression as a consequence of environmental (life) stressors (see Caspi et al., 2003, "Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene," Science 301: 386-89).
Dr. Moffitt has graciously provided us with three points in rebuttal to the conclusions of the Risch study, set forth in her own words:
1. The JAMA article ignores the wider body of scientific evidence. In the past 6 years, extensive research in experimental neuroscience using both animals and humans has validated the original report by showing that 5-HTTLPR short allele-carriers are excessively vulnerable to stress. Experimental studies that expose human participants to stressors in the laboratory show that individuals having the 5-HTTLPR short genotype have greater stress responses on measures of cognitive reactivity (e.g., Beevers et al., 2007, J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116: 208-12; Fox et al., 2009, Proc. Biol. Sci. 276: 1747-51), hormonal reactivity (e.g., Chen et al., 2009, Psychoneuroendocrinology 34: 681-86; Gotlib et al., 2008, Biol. Psychiatry. 63: 847-51), physiological reactivity (e.g., Lonsdorf et al., 2009, Psychol. Sci. 20: 198-206) and reactivity in the brain's emotion-circuitry (e.g., Hariri et al., 2006, Arch. Gen. Psych. 62: 146-52; Canli et al. 2006, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103: 16033-38). This vulnerability of 5-HTTLPR S carriers has also been confirmed in animal research (e.g., Carola et al., 2008, Biol. Psychiatry. 63: 840-46; Barr et al., 2004, Biol. Psych. 55: 733-38; Kalin et al., 2008, Mol. Psychiatry. 13: 1021-27, online at DOI: 10.1038/mp.2008.37; Watson et al., 2009, PLoS One, published online January 14, 2009). Further validation comes from studies which have shown that 5-HTTLPR Short allele carriers are vulnerable not only to depression, but also to other mental-health problems caused by stress, including PTSD and anxiety (e.g., Gunthert et al., 2007, Psychosom. Med. 69: 762-68; Kilpatrick et al., 2007, Am. J. Psychiatry. 164: 1693-99).
2. The selection of studies for meta-analysis clearly fails to represent the pool of papers in the literature. 4 out of 17 positive replications were included (24%) but 6 out of 9 published negative
studies (67%), a bias that violates a basic requirement of the meta-analysis method to examine a representative sample of the existing literature. Further, the article opted not to analyze several well-designed studies of individuals exposed to stress. For example, studies of children who are victims of abuse (e.g., Cicchetti et al., 2007, Dev. Psychopathol. 19: 1161-80; Kaufman et al., 2004, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101: 17316-21) and patients suffering hip fractures, strokes and coronary heart disease (Kohen et al., 2008, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 65: 1296-302; Lenze et al., 2008, Amer. J. Geriatric Psych. 13: 428-32; Otte et al., 2007, Am. J. Psychiatry. 164: 1379-84) have reported positive findings for this "GxE" hypothesis. Several other positive replications from studies with very strong research designs were omitted from the meta-analysis as well (Kendler et al., 2005, Arch. Gen. Psych. 62: 529-35; Drachmann-Bukh et al., 2009, J. Affect. Disord. 2009 Mar. 30 (e-publ. ahead of print at doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.02.023); Lazary et al., 2008, Biol. Psychiatry. 64: 498-504).
3. Within the smaller subset of studies in the meta-analysis, there is important heterogeneity. The article says this heterogeneity was not statistically significant. However, we have learned that the test fell just short of statistical significance. One obvious characteristic that varies widely across studies in the meta-analysis is measurement quality. As one example, meta-analysis gives more mathematical weight to studies with the largest samples. But in this case the big studies had to collect their data through telephone calls or self-complete postal questionnaires, which are known to be weak methods of measuring life events and depression. Not surprisingly, these big studies with weak measures tended not to find positive results, tilting the meta-analysis toward a null finding. In the existing literature of tests of this GxE hypothesis, 13 studies have replicated the finding, using measures collected in face-to-face clinical assessments, and 10 studies failed to replicate the finding, using measures collected via phone or postal questionnaires. This clear methodological pattern was ignored in the meta-analysis. This is one example of heterogeneity could have been followed up in a meta-analysis, there may be others.
Dr. Moffitt concluded her response to Dr. Risch's work by noting that "[w]hat is needed is not less research into gene-environment interaction, as Risch et al. recommend, but more research of better quality, and a more thorough and thoughtful evaluation of it."
Dr. Moffitt has extensive experience in studying the biological basis of complex behaviors, and in addition to her Duke University appointment is also Professor of Social Behaviour and Development, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, London and Associate Director, Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Dunedin School of Medicine, Dunedin, New Zealand.
This controversy illustrates one of the many pitfalls frequently faced by academic inventors motivated by the dual (and occasionally conflicting) needs of disseminating the results of their studies as rapidly as possible and protecting the intellectual property aspects of their work to maximize the likelihood that it can be commercialized (and not merely expropriated by others based on its disclosure in the academic literature). As a result, often the research is not as developed as it might be if it were developed by a commercial concern initially. This situation presents particular challenges to university technology transfer offices and their clients, university faculty and university administrations. It is an imperfect system, which motivates some to say there is a flaw in the basic premise of promoting university patenting under the Bayh-Dole regime. However, the alternative (not protecting university-generated inventions) is indeed worse, since then the public stands no chance of seeing the fruits of these labors helping to finance the next generation of technologies.
"In the existing literature of tests of this GxE hypothesis, 13 studies have replicated the finding, using measures collected in face-to-face clinical assessments, and 10 studies failed to replicate the finding, using measures collected via phone or postal questionnaires. "
Without having looked at this in detail, wouldn't such a differnce indicate that clinicians tend to diagnose people carrying the GxE allele as depressed, while the people themselves feel "non-depressed"?
Posted by: Solli | June 23, 2009 at 03:36 AM
Dear Solti:
I think it may be that many people are more comfortable downplaying disorders like depression when the interview is done impersonally (on the telephone), whereas in-person interviews provide the researcher with a great deal more contextual and other non-verbal cues and other evidence. Most researchers would find the in-person research more reliable for these reasons.
Thanks for the comment.
Posted by: Kevin E. Noonan | June 23, 2009 at 07:11 AM
Dear Kevin,
Interesting tidbit from a purely scientific perspective. I agree with you; I tend to believe that face to face interviews are qualitatively different (more objective) than telephone or written questionnaires, when mental state is at issue - something about a potentially pathologically affected organ evaluating itself for pathologies.
I also note that the filing of patent applications was not mentioned in this case. I think papers describing conflicting results often are as much the result of academic rivalry and the need to "be first" with relatively raw data as anything else. Certainly, it would be worthwhile in such a case to first file provisional applications based on the relatively raw data with an eye to confirming the results after the paper has been submitted (hopefully before the MS is published) and before filing the non-provisional. Obviously even better to confirm the results before submitting the MS for publication at all!
Thanks again.
Posted by: Carlos A. Fisher | June 23, 2009 at 11:45 AM
Dear Carlos:
While I don't know the details, it would surprise me if that isn't just what happened for the patents based on this invention. The problem is that these types of studies take time, and so the inventor is left in the difficult position of filing on what the data tells her now, and hoping it is correct, or going through patent prosecution and having this type of study come out (although as we said in the original piece, it could not have come at a worse time).
My further understanding is that the type of nay-saying over the results found in the Risch study is not new, and that the inventors addressed other negative reports during prosecution. The Risch paper seems the most comprehensive, but as you can see the inventors have a position on the reliability of the Risch analysis.
Ultimately, the 5-HTT promoter polymorphism either will or will not be a predictor of depression risk assessment. Whether there is commercial value in such a claim is another question.
Thanks for the comment.
Posted by: Kevin E. Noonan | June 24, 2009 at 06:34 AM